Figure 1: The STEM Gender Equality Paradox
Source: Stoet and Geary (2018)
Why is this?
This idea, sadly, has some precedent. In January 2005 Lawrence
Summers – Economics Nobel laureate and then-President of Harvard University – received
heavy backlash after claiming that lack of female representation in STEM could
be caused by ‘natural differences in high-end aptitude’ between the genders, prompting
a media storm and his eventual resignation two months later.
Needless to say, there is very little evidence to support
Summers’ view. In looking at numeracy test scores across 67 countries and
regions, the Stoet and Geary paper found that girls performed just as well or better than boys in most countries, and
in almost all countries girls would have been capable of college-level STEM classes
had they enrolled in them.
A better explanation, put forward by the authors themselves,
suggests that men have a comparative but not absolute
advantage in STEM subjects. In all countries except Romania and Lebanon, the
researchers find that boys’ best subject was science and girls’ best subject
was reading. That is, girls performed just as well or better in science – they
just happened to perform even better in
reading.
Moreover – and here’s the kicker – the researchers find that
the more gender-equal the country,
the larger this gap between boys and girls in having
science as their best subject. This completes
the explanation: as countries get more gender-equal, boys’ comparative
advantage in STEM increases, widening the STEM gender-gap at University.
Stoet and Geary also put
forward another explanation that invokes life quality pressures. In less developed
and more gender-unequal countries, women face greater economic and life risks.
As a result, those fortunate enough to obtain a tertiary education are more
likely to choose STEM subjects, under the (generally well-founded) belief that
these are more likely to lead to a secure and lucrative job.
The authors then argue that these
financial considerations become less burdensome as countries develop, giving people
greater freedom to pursue their true interests. One of their findings supports
this: even when girls’ ability in science in school equalled or excelled that
of boys, they nevertheless tended to register a lower interest in science subjects. This explanation has a strangely
uplifting implication: it’s not that gender equality discourages girls from
pursuing science; it’s that it allows them not to if they’re not interested.
Outstanding Issues
I find both these
explanations unsatisfying. At its heart, the life pressures explanation rests on
the idea that men are naturally more interested in science than women – an idea
with very little intuitive appeal. Indeed, by invoking the idea of an ‘interest
gap’, all we seem to do is pass the buck on to another, no-less tractable
problem.
Likewise, the comparative
advantage explanation relies on the idea that greater gender-equality leads to
more pronounced relative advantages for women in non-STEM fields. Again, there
is no obviously intuitive explanation for why this might be the case.
More fundamentally, though,
both explanations seem to overlook a large elephant in the room. In emphasising
a benign sorting process based on interest or comparative skill, both
explanations conveniently side-step the numerous structural barriers that
prospective female STEM students face in countries we otherwise consider
relatively gender-equal. These barriers are extremely well-documented: in
recounting her experience as a woman in STEM, Ellen Pao describes the
accumulation of micro-aggressions as like “a thousand tiny papercuts”. Given
the pervasiveness of these barriers, any explanation which stays silent on them
seems patently incomplete.
Why Does This Matter?
If progress towards economic growth and gender-equality
comes at the expense of equal STEM gender-ratios, this has a number of unsettling
implications. First, gender STEM ratios today are likely to heavily influence the
gender composition of tech companies tomorrow. This matters because – as recent research shows – algorithms routinely inherit the biases of their creators. I
like the example Lukas gives: many ‘smart’ heating systems take the metabolic
heartrate of a middle-aged man as the default, making the entire algorithm
geared towards maximising the experience of this demographic. If more women
were included in the design process, this unfair gender-bias may have been
noted earlier and rectified.
We can push the envelope further. As Yuval Harari and Jamie
Susskind recently argue, the commodification of personal data is likely to
progressively shift power towards tech companies away from traditional sources
of authority, like nation states and religious institutions. The future leaders
of these companies are likely being educated today – in STEM lecture halls marked
by a conspicuous absence of females. If we care about the continuation of
patriarchal power structures, we need to take the STEM paradox seriously.
Word count: 954 words
References
- Lukas’ blog post: https://lukasfreund.wordpress.com/2018/08/05/gender-equality-and-stem-gaps/
- Stoet and Geary (2018), ‘The Gender-Equality Paradox in Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics Education’
For a great discussion of the implications of tech on the
future of politics and power, I would recommend:
- Harari (2015), ‘Homo Deus’
- Susskind (2018), ‘Future Politics’
No comments:
Post a Comment